What a character!
One significant advantage engineering, physics and much of the natural sciences has is that they speak in mathematical terms. The individuals often speak in formulas or mathematical verbiage that is clear and concise and understandable by all the members. The use of every day words like habit can be confusing because of a lack of clarity.
What is character? Character is the network of habits that permeate all the intentional acts of an individual.
I am not using the word habit in the way we often do, as a technical ability existing apart from our wishes. These habits are an intimate and fundamental part of our selves. They are representations of our will. They rule our will, working in a coordinated way they dominate our way of acting. These habits are the results of repeated, intelligently controlled, actions.
Habits also control the formation of ideas as well as physical actions. We cannot perform a correct action or a correct idea without having already formed correct habits. “Reason pure of all influence from prior habit is a fiction.
“The medium of habit filters all material that reaches our perception and thought. “Immediate, seemingly instinctive, feeling of the direction and end of various lines of behavior is in reality the feeling of habits working below direct consciousness. “Habit means special sensitiveness or accessibility to certain classes of stimuli, standing predilections and aversions, rather than bare recurrence of specific acts. It means will.
Because each job requires a different type of character a journalist would make a lousy military officer and vice versa.
Quotes from “Human Nature and Conduct by John Dewey
What a character!
What a character!
What's "character?" Read this....
by Carl Zimmer
Are brain parasites altering the personalities of three
billion people? The
question emerged a few
years ago, and it shows
no signs of going away.
I first encountered this
idea while working on my
book Parasite Rex. I was
investigating the
remarkable ability
parasites have to
manipulate the behavior
of their hosts. The
lancet fluke
Dicrocoelium
dendriticum, for
example, forces its ant
host to clamp itself to
the tip of grass blades,
where a grazing mammal
might eat it. It's in
the fluke's interest to
get eaten, because only
by getting into the gut
of a sheep or some other
grazer can it complete
its life cycle. Another
fluke, Euhaplorchis
californiensis, causes
infected fish to shimmy
and jump, greatly
increasing the chance
that wading birds will
grab them.
Those parasites were
weird enough, but then I
got to know Toxoplasma
gondii. This
single-celled parasite
lives in the guts of
cats, sheddding eggs
that can be picked up by
rats and other animals
that can just so happen
be eaten by cats.
Toxoplasma forms cysts
throughout its
intermediate host's
body, including the
brain. And yet a
Toxoplasma-ridden rat is
perfectly healthy. That
makes good sense for the
parasite, since a cat
would not be
particularly interested
in eating a dead rat.
But scientists at Oxford
discovered that the
parasite changes the
rats in one subtle but
vital way.
The scientists studied
the rats in a six-foot
by six-foot outdoor
enclosure. They used
bricks to turn it into a
maze of paths and cells.
In each corner of the
enclosure they put a
nest box along with a
bowl of food and water.
On each the nests they
added a few drops of a
particular odor. On one
they added the scent of
fresh straw bedding, on
another the bedding from
a rat's nests, on
another the scent of
rabbit urine, on
another, the urine of a
cat. When they set
healthy rats loose in
the enclosure, the
animals rooted around
curiously and
investigated the nests.
But when they came
across the cat odor,
they shied away and
never returned to that
corner. This was no
surprise: the odor of a
cat triggers a sudden
shift in the chemistry
of rat brains that
brings on intense
anxiety. (When
researchers test
anti-anxiety drugs on
rats, they use a whiff
of cat urine to make
them panic.) The anxiety
attack made the healthy
rats shy away from the
odor and in general
makes them leery of
investigating new
things. Better to lie
low and stay alive.
Then the researchers put
Toxoplasma-carrying rats
in the enclosure. Rats
carrying the parasite
are for the most part
indistinguishable from
healthy ones. They can
compete for mates just
as well and have no
trouble feeding
themselves. The only
difference, the
researchers found, is
that they are more
likely to get themselves
killed. The scent of a
cat in the enclosure
didn't make them
anxious, and they went
about their business as
if nothing was bothering
them. They would explore
around the odor at least
as often as they did
anywhere else in the
enclosure. In some
cases, they even took a
special interest in the
spot and came back to it
over and over again.
The scientists
speculated that
Toxoplasma was secreted
some substance that was
altering the patterns of
brain activity in the
rats. This manipulation
likely evolved through
natural selection, since
parasites that were more
likely to end up in cats
would leave more
offpsring.
The Oxford scientists
knew that humans can be
hosts to Toxoplasma,
too. People can become
infected by its eggs by
handling soil or kitty
litter. For most people,
the infection causes no
harm. Only if a person's
immune system is weak
does Toxoplasma grow
uncontrollably. That's
why pregnant women are
advised not to handle
kitty litter, and why
toxoplasmosis is a
serious risk for people
with AIDS. Otherwise,
the parasite lives
quietly in people's
bodies (and brains).
It's estimated that
about half of all people
on Earth are infected
with Toxoplasma.
Given that human and rat
brains have a lot of
similarities (they share
the same basic anatomy
and use the same
neurotransmitters), a
question naturally
arose: if Toxoplasma can
alter the behavior of a
rat, could it alter a
human? Obviously, this
manipulation would not
do the parasite any good
as an adaptation, since
it's pretty rare for a
human to be devoured by
a cat. But it could
still have an effect.
Some scientists believe
that Toxoplasma changes
the personality of its
human hosts, bringing
different shifts to men
and women.
Parasitologist Jaroslav
Flegr of Charles
University in Prague
administered
psychological
questionnaires to people
infected with Toxoplasma
and controls. Those
infected, he found, show
a small, but
statistically
significant, tendency to
be more self-reproaching
and insecure.
Paradoxically, infected
women, on average, tend
to be more outgoing and
warmhearted than
controls, while infected
men tend to be more
jealous and suspicious.
It's controversial work,
disputed by many. But it
attracted the attention
of E. Fuller Torrey of
the Stanley Medical
Research Institute in
Bethesda, Maryland.
Torrey and his
colleagues had noticed
some intriguing links
between Toxoplasma and
schizophrenia. Infection
with the parasite has
been associated with
damage to a certain
class of neurons
(astrocytes). So has
schizophrenia. Pregnant
women with high levels
of Toxoplasma antibodies
in their blood were more
likely to give birth to
children who would later
develop schizophrenia.
Torrey lays out more
links in this 2003 paper
. While none is a
smoking gun, they are
certainly food for
thought. It's
conceivable that
exposure to Toxoplasma
causes subtle changes in
most people's
personality, but in a
small minority, it has
more devastating
effects.
A year later, Torrey and
his colleagues
discovered one more
fascinating link. They
raised human cells in
Petri dishes and
infected them with
Toxoplasma. Then they
dosed the cells with a
variety of drugs used to
treat schizophrenia.
Several of the
drugs--most notably
haloperidol--blocked the
growth of the parasite.
So Fuller and the Oxford
scientists joined forces
to find an answer to the
next logical question:
can drugs used to treat
schizophrenia help a
parasite-crazed rat?
They now report their
results in the
Proceedings of the Royal
Society of London (press
release). They ran the
original tests on 49
more rats. Once again,
parasitized rats lost
their healthy fear of
cats. Then the
researchers treated the
rats with haloperidol
and several other
anti-psychotic drugs.
They found that the
drugs made the rats more
scared. They also found
that the antipsychotics
were as effective as
pyrimethamine, a drug
that is specifically
used to eliminate
Toxoplasma.
There's plenty left to
do to turn these results
into a full-blown
explanation of parasites
and personalities. For
example, what is
Toxoplasma releasing
into brains to
manipulate its hosts?
And how does that
substance give rise to
schizophrenia in some
humans? And even if the
hypothesis does hold up,
it would only account
for some cases of
schizophrenia, while the
cause of others would
remain undiscovered. But
still...the idea that
parasites are tinkering
with humanity's
personality--perhaps
even giving rise to
cultural diversity--is
taking over my head like
a bad case of
Toxoplasma.
(Sorry for the format....I couldn't seem to fix it.)
by Carl Zimmer
Are brain parasites altering the personalities of three
billion people? The
question emerged a few
years ago, and it shows
no signs of going away.
I first encountered this
idea while working on my
book Parasite Rex. I was
investigating the
remarkable ability
parasites have to
manipulate the behavior
of their hosts. The
lancet fluke
Dicrocoelium
dendriticum, for
example, forces its ant
host to clamp itself to
the tip of grass blades,
where a grazing mammal
might eat it. It's in
the fluke's interest to
get eaten, because only
by getting into the gut
of a sheep or some other
grazer can it complete
its life cycle. Another
fluke, Euhaplorchis
californiensis, causes
infected fish to shimmy
and jump, greatly
increasing the chance
that wading birds will
grab them.
Those parasites were
weird enough, but then I
got to know Toxoplasma
gondii. This
single-celled parasite
lives in the guts of
cats, sheddding eggs
that can be picked up by
rats and other animals
that can just so happen
be eaten by cats.
Toxoplasma forms cysts
throughout its
intermediate host's
body, including the
brain. And yet a
Toxoplasma-ridden rat is
perfectly healthy. That
makes good sense for the
parasite, since a cat
would not be
particularly interested
in eating a dead rat.
But scientists at Oxford
discovered that the
parasite changes the
rats in one subtle but
vital way.
The scientists studied
the rats in a six-foot
by six-foot outdoor
enclosure. They used
bricks to turn it into a
maze of paths and cells.
In each corner of the
enclosure they put a
nest box along with a
bowl of food and water.
On each the nests they
added a few drops of a
particular odor. On one
they added the scent of
fresh straw bedding, on
another the bedding from
a rat's nests, on
another the scent of
rabbit urine, on
another, the urine of a
cat. When they set
healthy rats loose in
the enclosure, the
animals rooted around
curiously and
investigated the nests.
But when they came
across the cat odor,
they shied away and
never returned to that
corner. This was no
surprise: the odor of a
cat triggers a sudden
shift in the chemistry
of rat brains that
brings on intense
anxiety. (When
researchers test
anti-anxiety drugs on
rats, they use a whiff
of cat urine to make
them panic.) The anxiety
attack made the healthy
rats shy away from the
odor and in general
makes them leery of
investigating new
things. Better to lie
low and stay alive.
Then the researchers put
Toxoplasma-carrying rats
in the enclosure. Rats
carrying the parasite
are for the most part
indistinguishable from
healthy ones. They can
compete for mates just
as well and have no
trouble feeding
themselves. The only
difference, the
researchers found, is
that they are more
likely to get themselves
killed. The scent of a
cat in the enclosure
didn't make them
anxious, and they went
about their business as
if nothing was bothering
them. They would explore
around the odor at least
as often as they did
anywhere else in the
enclosure. In some
cases, they even took a
special interest in the
spot and came back to it
over and over again.
The scientists
speculated that
Toxoplasma was secreted
some substance that was
altering the patterns of
brain activity in the
rats. This manipulation
likely evolved through
natural selection, since
parasites that were more
likely to end up in cats
would leave more
offpsring.
The Oxford scientists
knew that humans can be
hosts to Toxoplasma,
too. People can become
infected by its eggs by
handling soil or kitty
litter. For most people,
the infection causes no
harm. Only if a person's
immune system is weak
does Toxoplasma grow
uncontrollably. That's
why pregnant women are
advised not to handle
kitty litter, and why
toxoplasmosis is a
serious risk for people
with AIDS. Otherwise,
the parasite lives
quietly in people's
bodies (and brains).
It's estimated that
about half of all people
on Earth are infected
with Toxoplasma.
Given that human and rat
brains have a lot of
similarities (they share
the same basic anatomy
and use the same
neurotransmitters), a
question naturally
arose: if Toxoplasma can
alter the behavior of a
rat, could it alter a
human? Obviously, this
manipulation would not
do the parasite any good
as an adaptation, since
it's pretty rare for a
human to be devoured by
a cat. But it could
still have an effect.
Some scientists believe
that Toxoplasma changes
the personality of its
human hosts, bringing
different shifts to men
and women.
Parasitologist Jaroslav
Flegr of Charles
University in Prague
administered
psychological
questionnaires to people
infected with Toxoplasma
and controls. Those
infected, he found, show
a small, but
statistically
significant, tendency to
be more self-reproaching
and insecure.
Paradoxically, infected
women, on average, tend
to be more outgoing and
warmhearted than
controls, while infected
men tend to be more
jealous and suspicious.
It's controversial work,
disputed by many. But it
attracted the attention
of E. Fuller Torrey of
the Stanley Medical
Research Institute in
Bethesda, Maryland.
Torrey and his
colleagues had noticed
some intriguing links
between Toxoplasma and
schizophrenia. Infection
with the parasite has
been associated with
damage to a certain
class of neurons
(astrocytes). So has
schizophrenia. Pregnant
women with high levels
of Toxoplasma antibodies
in their blood were more
likely to give birth to
children who would later
develop schizophrenia.
Torrey lays out more
links in this 2003 paper
. While none is a
smoking gun, they are
certainly food for
thought. It's
conceivable that
exposure to Toxoplasma
causes subtle changes in
most people's
personality, but in a
small minority, it has
more devastating
effects.
A year later, Torrey and
his colleagues
discovered one more
fascinating link. They
raised human cells in
Petri dishes and
infected them with
Toxoplasma. Then they
dosed the cells with a
variety of drugs used to
treat schizophrenia.
Several of the
drugs--most notably
haloperidol--blocked the
growth of the parasite.
So Fuller and the Oxford
scientists joined forces
to find an answer to the
next logical question:
can drugs used to treat
schizophrenia help a
parasite-crazed rat?
They now report their
results in the
Proceedings of the Royal
Society of London (press
release). They ran the
original tests on 49
more rats. Once again,
parasitized rats lost
their healthy fear of
cats. Then the
researchers treated the
rats with haloperidol
and several other
anti-psychotic drugs.
They found that the
drugs made the rats more
scared. They also found
that the antipsychotics
were as effective as
pyrimethamine, a drug
that is specifically
used to eliminate
Toxoplasma.
There's plenty left to
do to turn these results
into a full-blown
explanation of parasites
and personalities. For
example, what is
Toxoplasma releasing
into brains to
manipulate its hosts?
And how does that
substance give rise to
schizophrenia in some
humans? And even if the
hypothesis does hold up,
it would only account
for some cases of
schizophrenia, while the
cause of others would
remain undiscovered. But
still...the idea that
parasites are tinkering
with humanity's
personality--perhaps
even giving rise to
cultural diversity--is
taking over my head like
a bad case of
Toxoplasma.
(Sorry for the format....I couldn't seem to fix it.)
My candle's burning at both ends, it will not last the night. But ah, my foes, and oh, my friends--It gives a lovely light!--Edna St. Vincent Millay
- Accountable
- Posts: 24818
- Joined: Mon May 30, 2005 8:33 am
What a character!
Lulu2 wrote: What's "character?" Read this....by Carl Zimmer
Are brain parasites altering the personalities of three billion people? The question emerged a few years ago, and it shows no signs of going away.
I first encountered this idea while working on my book Parasite Rex. I was investigating the remarkable ability parasites have to manipulate the behavior
of their hosts. The lancet fluke Dicrocoelium dendriticum, for example, forces its ant host to clamp itself to the tip of grass blades, where a grazing mammal might eat it. It's in the fluke's interest to get eaten, because only by getting into the gut of a sheep or some other grazer can it complete its life cycle. Another fluke, Euhaplorchis californiensis, causes infected fish to shimmy and jump, greatly increasing the chance that wading birds will grab them.
Those parasites were weird enough, but then I got to know Toxoplasma gondii. This single-celled parasite lives in the guts of cats, sheddding eggs that can be picked up by rats and other animals that can just so happen be eaten by cats. Toxoplasma forms cysts throughout its intermediate host's body, including the brain. And yet a Toxoplasma-ridden rat is perfectly healthy. That makes good sense for the parasite, since a cat would not be particularly interested in eating a dead rat. But scientists at Oxford discovered that the parasite changes the rats in one subtle but vital way.
The scientists studied the rats in a six-foot by six-foot outdoor enclosure. They used bricks to turn it into a maze of paths and cells. In each corner of the enclosure they put a nest box along with a bowl of food and water. On each the nests they added a few drops of a particular odor. On one they added the scent of fresh straw bedding, on another the bedding from a rat's nests, on another the scent of rabbit urine, on another, the urine of a cat. When they set healthy rats loose in the enclosure, the animals rooted around
curiously and investigated the nests. But when they came across the cat odor, they shied away and never returned to that corner. This was no surprise: the odor of a cat triggers a sudden shift in the chemistry of rat brains that brings on intense anxiety. (When researchers test anti-anxiety drugs on rats, they use a whiff of cat urine to make them panic.) The anxiety attack made the healthy rats shy away from the odor and in general makes them leery of investigating new things. Better to lie low and stay alive.
Then the researchers put Toxoplasma-carrying rats in the enclosure. Rats carrying the parasite are for the most part indistinguishable from healthy ones. They can compete for mates just as well and have no trouble feeding themselves. The only difference, the researchers found, is that they are more likely to get themselves killed. The scent of a cat in the enclosure didn't make them anxious, and they went about their business as if nothing was bothering them. They would explore around the odor at least as often as they did anywhere else in the enclosure. In some cases, they even took a special interest in the spot and came back to it over and over again.
The scientists speculated that Toxoplasma was secreted some substance that was altering the patterns of brain activity in the rats. This manipulation likely evolved through natural selection, since parasites that were more likely to end up in cats would leave more offpsring.
The Oxford scientists knew that humans can be hosts to Toxoplasma, too. People can become infected by its eggs by handling soil or kitty litter. For most people, the infection causes no harm. Only if a person's immune system is weak does Toxoplasma grow uncontrollably. That's why pregnant women are advised not to handle kitty litter, and why toxoplasmosis is a serious risk for people with AIDS. Otherwise, the parasite lives quietly in people's bodies (and brains). It's estimated that about half of all people on Earth are infected with Toxoplasma.
Given that human and rat brains have a lot of similarities (they share the same basic anatomy and use the same neurotransmitters), a question naturally arose: if Toxoplasma can alter the behavior of a rat, could it alter a human? Obviously, this manipulation would not do the parasite any good as an adaptation, since it's pretty rare for a human to be devoured by a cat. But it could still have an effect.
Some scientists believe that Toxoplasma changes the personality of its human hosts, bringing different shifts to men and women. Parasitologist Jaroslav Flegr of Charles University in Prague administered psychological
questionnaires to people infected with Toxoplasma and controls. Those infected, he found, show a small, but statistically significant, tendency to be more self-reproaching and insecure. Paradoxically, infected women, on average, tend to be more outgoing and warmhearted than controls, while infected men tend to be more jealous and suspicious.
It's controversial work, disputed by many. But it attracted the attention of E. Fuller Torrey of the Stanley Medical Research Institute in Bethesda, Maryland. Torrey and his colleagues had noticed some intriguing links between Toxoplasma and schizophrenia. Infection with the parasite has been associated with damage to a certain class of neurons (astrocytes). So has schizophrenia. Pregnant women with high levels of Toxoplasma antibodies in their blood were more likely to give birth to children who would later develop schizophrenia. Torrey lays out more links in this 2003 paper . While none is a smoking gun, they are certainly food for thought. It's conceivable that exposure to Toxoplasma causes subtle changes in most people's personality, but in a small minority, it has more devastating effects.
A year later, Torrey and his colleagues discovered one more fascinating link. They raised human cells in Petri dishes and infected them with Toxoplasma. Then they dosed the cells with a variety of drugs used to treat schizophrenia. Several of the drugs--most notably haloperidol--blocked the growth of the parasite.
So Fuller and the Oxford scientists joined forces to find an answer to the next logical question: can drugs used to treat schizophrenia help a parasite-crazed rat? They now report their results in the Proceedings of the Royal Society of London (press release). They ran the original tests on 49 more rats. Once again, parasitized rats lost their healthy fear of cats. Then the researchers treated the rats with haloperidol and several other anti-psychotic drugs. They found that the drugs made the rats more scared. They also found that the antipsychotics were as effective as pyrimethamine, a drug that is specifically used to eliminate Toxoplasma.
There's plenty left to do to turn these results into a full-blown explanation of parasites and personalities. For example, what is Toxoplasma releasing into brains to manipulate its hosts? And how does that substance give rise to schizophrenia in some humans? And even if the hypothesis does hold up, it would only account for some cases of schizophrenia, while the cause of others would remain undiscovered. But still...the idea that parasites are tinkering with humanity's personality--perhaps even giving rise to cultural diversity--is taking over my head like a bad case of Toxoplasma.
(Sorry for the format....I couldn't seem to fix it.)I thought it would help me read it, but it became only a challenge to conquer.
Are brain parasites altering the personalities of three billion people? The question emerged a few years ago, and it shows no signs of going away.
I first encountered this idea while working on my book Parasite Rex. I was investigating the remarkable ability parasites have to manipulate the behavior
of their hosts. The lancet fluke Dicrocoelium dendriticum, for example, forces its ant host to clamp itself to the tip of grass blades, where a grazing mammal might eat it. It's in the fluke's interest to get eaten, because only by getting into the gut of a sheep or some other grazer can it complete its life cycle. Another fluke, Euhaplorchis californiensis, causes infected fish to shimmy and jump, greatly increasing the chance that wading birds will grab them.
Those parasites were weird enough, but then I got to know Toxoplasma gondii. This single-celled parasite lives in the guts of cats, sheddding eggs that can be picked up by rats and other animals that can just so happen be eaten by cats. Toxoplasma forms cysts throughout its intermediate host's body, including the brain. And yet a Toxoplasma-ridden rat is perfectly healthy. That makes good sense for the parasite, since a cat would not be particularly interested in eating a dead rat. But scientists at Oxford discovered that the parasite changes the rats in one subtle but vital way.
The scientists studied the rats in a six-foot by six-foot outdoor enclosure. They used bricks to turn it into a maze of paths and cells. In each corner of the enclosure they put a nest box along with a bowl of food and water. On each the nests they added a few drops of a particular odor. On one they added the scent of fresh straw bedding, on another the bedding from a rat's nests, on another the scent of rabbit urine, on another, the urine of a cat. When they set healthy rats loose in the enclosure, the animals rooted around
curiously and investigated the nests. But when they came across the cat odor, they shied away and never returned to that corner. This was no surprise: the odor of a cat triggers a sudden shift in the chemistry of rat brains that brings on intense anxiety. (When researchers test anti-anxiety drugs on rats, they use a whiff of cat urine to make them panic.) The anxiety attack made the healthy rats shy away from the odor and in general makes them leery of investigating new things. Better to lie low and stay alive.
Then the researchers put Toxoplasma-carrying rats in the enclosure. Rats carrying the parasite are for the most part indistinguishable from healthy ones. They can compete for mates just as well and have no trouble feeding themselves. The only difference, the researchers found, is that they are more likely to get themselves killed. The scent of a cat in the enclosure didn't make them anxious, and they went about their business as if nothing was bothering them. They would explore around the odor at least as often as they did anywhere else in the enclosure. In some cases, they even took a special interest in the spot and came back to it over and over again.
The scientists speculated that Toxoplasma was secreted some substance that was altering the patterns of brain activity in the rats. This manipulation likely evolved through natural selection, since parasites that were more likely to end up in cats would leave more offpsring.
The Oxford scientists knew that humans can be hosts to Toxoplasma, too. People can become infected by its eggs by handling soil or kitty litter. For most people, the infection causes no harm. Only if a person's immune system is weak does Toxoplasma grow uncontrollably. That's why pregnant women are advised not to handle kitty litter, and why toxoplasmosis is a serious risk for people with AIDS. Otherwise, the parasite lives quietly in people's bodies (and brains). It's estimated that about half of all people on Earth are infected with Toxoplasma.
Given that human and rat brains have a lot of similarities (they share the same basic anatomy and use the same neurotransmitters), a question naturally arose: if Toxoplasma can alter the behavior of a rat, could it alter a human? Obviously, this manipulation would not do the parasite any good as an adaptation, since it's pretty rare for a human to be devoured by a cat. But it could still have an effect.
Some scientists believe that Toxoplasma changes the personality of its human hosts, bringing different shifts to men and women. Parasitologist Jaroslav Flegr of Charles University in Prague administered psychological
questionnaires to people infected with Toxoplasma and controls. Those infected, he found, show a small, but statistically significant, tendency to be more self-reproaching and insecure. Paradoxically, infected women, on average, tend to be more outgoing and warmhearted than controls, while infected men tend to be more jealous and suspicious.
It's controversial work, disputed by many. But it attracted the attention of E. Fuller Torrey of the Stanley Medical Research Institute in Bethesda, Maryland. Torrey and his colleagues had noticed some intriguing links between Toxoplasma and schizophrenia. Infection with the parasite has been associated with damage to a certain class of neurons (astrocytes). So has schizophrenia. Pregnant women with high levels of Toxoplasma antibodies in their blood were more likely to give birth to children who would later develop schizophrenia. Torrey lays out more links in this 2003 paper . While none is a smoking gun, they are certainly food for thought. It's conceivable that exposure to Toxoplasma causes subtle changes in most people's personality, but in a small minority, it has more devastating effects.
A year later, Torrey and his colleagues discovered one more fascinating link. They raised human cells in Petri dishes and infected them with Toxoplasma. Then they dosed the cells with a variety of drugs used to treat schizophrenia. Several of the drugs--most notably haloperidol--blocked the growth of the parasite.
So Fuller and the Oxford scientists joined forces to find an answer to the next logical question: can drugs used to treat schizophrenia help a parasite-crazed rat? They now report their results in the Proceedings of the Royal Society of London (press release). They ran the original tests on 49 more rats. Once again, parasitized rats lost their healthy fear of cats. Then the researchers treated the rats with haloperidol and several other anti-psychotic drugs. They found that the drugs made the rats more scared. They also found that the antipsychotics were as effective as pyrimethamine, a drug that is specifically used to eliminate Toxoplasma.
There's plenty left to do to turn these results into a full-blown explanation of parasites and personalities. For example, what is Toxoplasma releasing into brains to manipulate its hosts? And how does that substance give rise to schizophrenia in some humans? And even if the hypothesis does hold up, it would only account for some cases of schizophrenia, while the cause of others would remain undiscovered. But still...the idea that parasites are tinkering with humanity's personality--perhaps even giving rise to cultural diversity--is taking over my head like a bad case of Toxoplasma.
(Sorry for the format....I couldn't seem to fix it.)I thought it would help me read it, but it became only a challenge to conquer.

What a character!
??????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!
- Accountable
- Posts: 24818
- Joined: Mon May 30, 2005 8:33 am
What a character!
coberst wrote: ???????????????????????????????????????????????????????????
??????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????
?????????????????????????????????????????????????????!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!
!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!
!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!(sorry, I broke up your post so it wouldn't blow the margins)
I have the same comment for your opening post, coberst. Character is not behavior. Character is reflected in behavior. It is formed just as the habits are formed, but can't be performed.
??????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????????
?????????????????????????????????????????????????????!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!
!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!
!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!(sorry, I broke up your post so it wouldn't blow the margins)
I have the same comment for your opening post, coberst. Character is not behavior. Character is reflected in behavior. It is formed just as the habits are formed, but can't be performed.
What a character!
Accountable wrote: (sorry, I broke up your post so it wouldn't blow the margins)
I have the same comment for your opening post, coberst. Character is not behavior. Character is reflected in behavior. It is formed just as the habits are formed, but can't be performed.
You are correct. I misspoke.
I have the same comment for your opening post, coberst. Character is not behavior. Character is reflected in behavior. It is formed just as the habits are formed, but can't be performed.
You are correct. I misspoke.